Page 140 - 2024-bfw-MyersAP4e
P. 140

Responding to Loud and Soft Sounds
                                                How do we detect loudness? If you guessed that it’s related to the intensity of a hair cell’s
                                                response, you’d be wrong. Rather, a soft, pure tone activates only the few hair cells attuned
                                                to its frequency. Given louder sounds, neighboring hair cells also respond. Thus, your brain
                                                interprets loudness from the number of activated hair cells.
                                                   If a hair cell loses sensitivity to soft sounds, it may still respond to loud sounds. This
                                                helps explain another surprise: Really loud sounds may seem loud to people with or with-
                                                out normal hearing. Given my hearing loss, I [DM] have wondered what really loud music
                                                must sound like to people with normal hearing. Now I realize it sounds much the same;
                                                where we differ is in our perception of soft sounds (and our ability to isolate one sound
                                 Distributed by Bedford, Freeman & Worth Publishers. Not for redistribution.
                                                amid noise).
                                                Hearing Different Pitches
                                                How do we know whether a sound is the high-frequency, high-pitched chirp of a bird or
                                           Copyright © Bedford, Freeman & Worth Publishers.
                                                the low-frequency, low-pitched roar of a truck? Current thinking on how we discriminate
                                                pitch combines two theories.
                                                •   Place theory (also called place coding) presumes that we hear different pitches because
                                                   different sound waves trigger activity at different places along the cochlea’s basilar
                                                   membrane. Thus, the brain determines a sound’s pitch by recognizing the specific place
                                                   (on the membrane) that is generating the neural signal. When Nobel laureate-to-be
                                                   Georg von Békésy (1957) cut holes in the cochleas of guinea pigs and human cadavers
                                                   and looked inside with a microscope, he discovered that the cochlea vibrated, rather like
                                                   a shaken bedsheet, in response to sound. High frequencies produced large vibrations
                                                   near the beginning of the cochlea’s membrane. Low frequencies vibrated more of the
                                                   membrane and were not so easily localized. So, there is a problem: Place theory can
                                                   explain how we hear high-pitched sounds but not low-pitched sounds.
                                                •   Frequency theory (also called  temporal coding) suggests another explanation that
                                                   accounts for our ability to hear low-pitched sounds: The brain reads pitch by monitor-
                                                   ing the frequency of neural impulses traveling up the auditory nerve. The whole basilar
                                                   membrane vibrates with the incoming sound wave, triggering neural impulses to the
                                                   brain at the same rate as the sound wave. If the sound wave has a frequency of 100 waves
                                                   per second, then 100 pulses per second travel up the auditory nerve. But frequency
                                                     theory also has a problem: An individual neuron cannot fire faster than 1000 times
                                                   per second. How, then, can we sense sounds with frequencies above 1000 waves per
                                                   second (roughly the upper third of a piano keyboard)? Enter volley theory: Like soldiers
                                                   who alternate firing so that some can shoot while others reload, neural cells can alter-
                                                   nate firing. By firing in rapid succession, they can achieve a combined frequency above
                                                   1000 waves per second.
                   place theory  in hearing, the   So, place theory and frequency theory work together to enable our perception of
                   theory that links the pitch we   pitch. Place theory best explains how we sense high pitches. Frequency theory, extended
                   hear with the place where    by volley theory, also explains how we sense low pitches. Finally, some combination of
                   the cochlea’s membrane is
                   stimulated. (Also called place   the place and frequency theories likely explains how we sense pitches in the  intermediate
                   coding.)                     range.
                   frequency theory  in hearing,
                   the theory that the rate of   Localizing Sounds
                   nerve impulses traveling up   Why don’t we have one big ear — perhaps above our one nose? “All the better to hear you
                   the auditory nerve matches
                   the frequency of a tone, thus   with,” as the wolf said to Little Red Riding Hood. Thanks to the placement of our two ears,
                   enabling us to sense its pitch.   we enjoy stereophonic (“three-dimensional”) hearing. Two ears are better than one for at
                   (Also called temporal coding.)  least two reasons (Figure 1.6-21). If a car to your right honks, your right ear will receive a
                                                more intense sound, and it will receive the sound slightly sooner than your left ear.


                 140   Unit 1  Biological Bases of Behavior






          03_myersAPpsychology4e_28116_ch01_002_163.indd   140                                                                  15/12/23   9:26 AM
   135   136   137   138   139   140   141   142   143   144   145